Response channel activation and the lateral prefrontal cortex

@article{Rafal1996ResponseCA,
  title={Response channel activation and the lateral prefrontal cortex},
  author={R. Rafal and F. Gershberg and R. Egly and R. Ivry and A. Kingstone and T. Ro},
  journal={Neuropsychologia},
  year={1996},
  volume={34},
  pages={1197-1202}
}
The role of lateral prefrontal cortex in transducing perception into action was studied in 10 patients with chronic, unilateral lesions. They identified colors in the center of a visual display, while a flanking, distractor color was presented simultaneously in either the ipsilesional or contralesional field. The flanker could be either the same color as the target, or incompatible with the correct response. The effects of compatible and incompatible flankers on reaction time (RT) served as a… Expand

Figures, Tables, and Topics from this paper

Response Channel Activation and the Temporoparietal Junction
TLDR
Results suggest that the TPJ, in combination with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, is involved in transducing perception into action. Expand
Neural Activation During Response Competition
TLDR
This study measured brain activity using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during performance of the flanker task to suggest that the frontal foci may be related to response inhibition processes whereas the posterior foci is related to the activation of representations of the inappropriate responses. Expand
Contributions of the human pulvinar to linking vision and action
TLDR
The main finding was that when target location was not known and target and flanker were associated with competing responses, reaction times to contralesional targets were longer than those to ipsilesional targets. Expand
Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection
TLDR
Findings support the idea that parietal cortex is involved in activating possible responses on the basis of learned stimulus-response associations, and that prefrontal cortex is recruited when there is a need to select between competing responses. Expand
Goal-directed selective attention and response competition monitoring: evidence from unilateral parietal and anterior cingulate lesions.
TLDR
Patients with a parietal lesion and an ACC lesion demonstrated reduced interference effects for contralesional flankers, suggesting that response competition monitoring may be subserved by an integrated neural network including the anterior cingulate cortex. Expand
Selective visual attention in patients with frontal lobelesions or Parkinsons disease
TLDR
The data suggest that the cognitive processes underlying selective attention are, in general, spared in patients with frontal lobe lesions or basal ganglia dysfunction. Expand
Posterior parietal cortex and the filtering of distractors
TLDR
Testing the ability of a patient with bilateral parietal lesions to discriminate the features of a target surrounded by distractors of variable contrast found a finding consistent with the projections of parietal cortex to visual processing areas covering a wide range of receptive field sizes and eccentricities. Expand
Temporal grouping modulates ipsilateral capture in right visual neglect
TLDR
The performance of a patient with right neglect across various manipulations of the flanker paradigm was assessed, with results attributed to ipsilesional capture in central target detection, which is offset by temporal grouping processes when another stimulus appears on the contralateral side. Expand
Asymmetries in a unilateral flanker task depend on the direction of the response: the role of attentional shift and perceptual grouping.
TLDR
Four experiments were conducted using a flanker task with 1 distractor appearing either on the left or right side of a central target, and participants were more likely to correctly recognize letters that were briefly presented at the distractor position on the same side as the response. Expand
Asymmetries in a unilateral flanker task depend on the direction of the response: the role of attentional shift and perceptual grouping.
Four experiments were conducted using a flanker task with 1 distractor appearing either on the left or right side of a central target. Responses were made on a keyboard aligned parallel to theExpand
...
1
2
3
4
5
...

References

SHOWING 1-6 OF 6 REFERENCES
Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention.
TLDR
Effects previously observed in the traditional experimental procedures were obtained, suggesting that the models developed (which propose inhibitory mechanisms, e.g.) apply to ecologically valid situations. Expand
Allocation of attention in the visual field.
  • C. Eriksen, Y. Yeh
  • Medicine
  • Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance
  • 1985
TLDR
Modifications in Jonides' (1983) two-process model were suggested in terms of a zoom lens model of attentional resources, conceived as capable of distribution over the visual field, but with low resolving power, or as continuously constricting to small portions of thevisual field with a concomitant increase in processing power. Expand
Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses.
TLDR
The results suggest that expectancies about the relative utility of the information extracted during the parallel and focused phases determine which phase is used to activate responses. Expand
Structure of the human brain : a photographic atlas
Using large-format cross-sectional photographs and corresponding labelled diagrams, this atlas builds a detailed picture of the human brain. Cross sections are cut both horizontally and at 20-30 DGExpand
Activating response codes by stimuli in the neglected visual field.
Defining functional cortical areas with "averaged" CT scans
  • Society Jor Neuroscience Abstracts
  • 1987