Motion and vision: why animals move their eyes

@article{Land1999MotionAV,
  title={Motion and vision: why animals move their eyes},
  author={Michael Francis Land},
  journal={Journal of Comparative Physiology A},
  year={1999},
  volume={185},
  pages={341-352}
}
  • M. Land
  • Published 1 October 1999
  • Biology
  • Journal of Comparative Physiology A
Abstract Nearly all animals with good vision have a repertoire of eye movements. The majority show a pattern of stable fixations with fast saccades that shift the direction of gaze. These movements may be made by the eyes themselves, or the head, or in some insects the whole body. The main reason for keeping gaze still during fixations is the need to avoid the blur that results from the long response time of the photoreceptors. Blur begins to degrade the image at a retinal velocity of about 1… 

Encoding of saccadic scene changes in the mouse retina

The results suggest that the global motion signals provide ‘spatial context’ to the response of the stimulus within the receptive field to support the idea that the retina actively encodes the saccade and may signal the abrupt scene change to downstream brain areas.

Eye Movements: Parameters, Mechanisms, and Active Vision

Analyzing the duration of fixations and the amplitude of saccades during everyday activities can reveal insights into the processing of visual information, allowing an understanding of what details of the environment receive attention.

Eye movements of vertebrates and their relation to eye form and function

  • M. Land
  • Biology, Psychology
    Journal of Comparative Physiology A
  • 2014
Eye movement use is related to the resolution distribution in the retina, with eyes that contain foveas, or areas of high ganglion cell density, being more likely to make targeting eye movements, not seen in animals with more uniform retinas.

Freely-moving mice visually pursue prey using a retinal area with least optic flow

This work developed a method to digitally reconstruct and quantify the visual scene of freely moving mice performing a visually based prey capture task and shows that the image of the prey is maintained within a small area, the functional focus, in the upper-temporal part of the retina.

Does retinal configuration make the head and eyes of foveate birds move?

The hypothesis that changes in retinal ganglion cell density from the retinal periphery to the center of acute vision-fovea would account for the inter-specific variation in avian head/eye movement behavior is tested.

Visual pursuit behavior in mice maintains the pursued prey on the retinal region with least optic flow

This method developed a method to digitally reconstruct and quantify the visual scene of freely moving mice performing a visually based prey capture task and shows that the prey image consistently falls within a small area of the VOR-stabilized visual field.

Motion perception of saccade-induced retinal translation

Evidence is provided that stimuli optimized for high-speed motion detection elicit clear motion perception against saccade direction, thus making the search for extraretinal suppression superfluous and suggesting that intrasaccadic motion perception is accomplished by motion-selective magnocellular neurons through temporal integration of rapidly increasing retinal velocities.

Muscles that move the retina augment compound eye vision in Drosophila.

It is shown that Drosophila use their retinal muscles to smoothly track visual motion, which helps to stabilize the retinal image, and also to perform small saccades when viewing a stationary scene, and that even the smallest retinal saccade activate visual neurons.

Dynamics of gaze control during prey capture in freely moving mice

It is found that the majority of eye movements are compensatory for head movements, thereby acting to stabilize the visual scene and help relate eye movements in the mouse to other species, and provide a foundation for studying active vision during ethological behaviors in themouse.

Design of A Saccadic Active Vision System

A saccadic vision system that uses a commercial, programmable image sensor mounted on a servomotor and CMOS image sensor design considerations for active vision applications, and methods to determine regions of interest and achieve high dynamic range on the sensor are described.
...

References

SHOWING 1-10 OF 58 REFERENCES

The Functions of Eye Movements in Animals Remote from Man

Visual acuity for moving objects in first- and second-order neurons of the fly compound eye.

The results indicate that responses in large monopolar cells to moving point objects are maximal at angular velocities of 100-200 degrees/s, and the resolution of point objects by both these early stages of the visual system is similar from stationary to an angular velocity of approximately 200 degrees /s, while resolution deteriorates approximately linearly with velocity.

Motion cues provide the bee's visual world with a third dimension

It is reported that bees can discriminate between objects at different distances irrespective of their size, mediated primarily by the green-sensitive visual channel and is therefore colour blind, like all of the motion-dependent behaviours investigated so far in the bee.

Visual Motion and Its Role in the Stabilization of Gaze

The author examines the role of Visual Motion in the Stabilization of Body Posture in Vertebrates and Vertebrate Retinal Ganglion Cells and the Optokinetic Response in Primates and its Possible Neuronal Substrate.

Prediction of maximum allowable retinal slip speed in the fiddler crab, Uca pugilator.

Two measurements of the temporal dynamics of photoreceptors in the fiddler crab, Uca pugilator: T, and the frequency response are presented and a conservative estimate of the highest retinal slip speed is allowed that does not decrease spatial acuity.

Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface.

  • D. H. Kelly
  • Engineering
    Journal of the Optical Society of America
  • 1979
The spatio-temporal threshold surface for stabilized vision is constructed, and its properties are displayed in terms of the usual frequency parameters; e.g., at low spatial frequencies, the temporal response becomes nearly independent of spatial frequency, while at low temporal frequency, the spatial response becomes independent of temporal frequency.

From insect vision to robot vision

The understanding of some invertebrate sensory-motor systems has now reached a level able to provide valuable design hints and this approach brings into prominence the mutual constraints in the designs of a sensory and a motor system, in both living and non-living ambulatory creatures.

SCANNING EYE MOVEMENTS IN A HETEROPOD MOLLUSC

The problem of the narrow field of view is solved in one heteropod species at least by systematic scanning movements of the eyes, in which each retina ' sweeps' through a 90 0 arc.

Structure and function of learning flights in bees and wasps

The consequences of the structure of learning flights for visual information processing are reviewed and how they may relate to the acquisition of a visual representation and the task of pinpointing the goal are discussed.
...