Manual Chronostasis Tactile Perception Precedes Physical Contact

@article{Yarrow2003ManualCT,
  title={Manual Chronostasis Tactile Perception Precedes Physical Contact},
  author={Kielan Yarrow and John C. Rothwell},
  journal={Current Biology},
  year={2003},
  volume={13},
  pages={1134-1139}
}

Figures from this paper

Space and Time in Perception and Action: Saccadic chronostasis and the continuity of subjective temporal experience across eye movements

This chapter discusses the preferred account of the Illusion, which posits that the onset of the postsaccadic stimulus is antedated to a moment just prior to movement initiation, and review supporting evidence that illustrates key characteristics of the illusion, including its dependency on saccade extent.

Active movement restores veridical event-timing after tactile adaptation.

It is shown that adaptation to tactile motion causes a strong contraction of the apparent duration of tactile stimuli, and it is suggested that active movement resets timing mechanisms in preparation for the new scenario that the movement will cause, eliminating inappropriate biases in perceived time.

Spatio-temporal contingency of saccade-induced chronostasis

It is shown that a clock positioned halfway between the initial fixation point and the saccade target does not exhibit prolongation of the interval following the sAccade, which suggests a local, object-specific mechanism underlying the stopped clock illusion that might originate from a shift in attention immediately preceding the eye movement.

Action, arousal, and subjective time

The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements

The findings suggest that saccadic chronostasis is not solely dependent on a backward shift in perceived saccade onset, but instead reflects a temporal dilation.

City Research Online Action, arousal and subjective time

Results indicated that the chronostasis effect is constant across a wide range of stimulus durations and does not reflect the pattern of visual stimulation experienced during a saccade, suggesting that arousal is not critical.

Consistent Chronostasis Effects across Saccade Categories Imply a Subcortical Efferent Trigger

It is suggested that chronostasis is triggered by a low-level signal arising in response to efferent signals generated in the superior colliculus, which is common to all the conditions tested and their respective neural circuits.

Spatio-temporal topography of saccadic overestimation of time

Taking a long look at action and time perception.

It is suggested that distortions of time perception related to an eye movement are not distinct from temporal distortions for other kinds of responses, and voluntary action is neither necessary nor sufficient for overestimation effects.

Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of Subjective Time Dilation

An event-related functional magnetic neuroimaging study on the time dilation effect revealed an increased activation of the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex in the context of self-referential processes.
...

References

SHOWING 1-10 OF 29 REFERENCES

Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity

It is suggested that temporal extension of the target's percept is one of the mechanisms that ‘fill in’ the perceptual ‘gap’ during saccadic suppression.

Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence

The results indicate that the illusion of chronostasis is not specific to saccadic eye movement, and may therefore involve a more general mechanism of how voluntary action influences time perception.

Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements.

1. The aim of this study was to demonstrate that goal-directed pointing movements, executed at normal speed to a small visual target, but without vision of the movement, do not rely on preprogrammed

Haptic localization and the internal representation of the hand in space

The analogous findings of experiments 1 and 2 indicate that the same inaccurate representation of dynamic hand position is used to both localize tactual stimuli in 3-D space and construct the perception of arm movement.

Subjective Referral of the Timing for a Conscious Sensory Experience

PREVIOUS studies had indicated that there is a substantial delay, up to about 0.5 s, before activity at cerebral levels achieves ‘neuronal adequacy’ for eliciting a conscious somatosensory experience

Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses.

It is suggested that the predictive visual mechanism is one in which the brain dynamically calculates the spatial location of objects in terms of desired displacement, which enables the oculomotor system to perform in a spatially accurate manner when there is a dissonance between the retinal location of a target and the saccade necessary to acquire that target.

Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: a functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man.

A modified hypothesis is proposed that for a peripheral sensory input, the primary evoked response of sensory cortex to the specific projection (lemniscal) input is associated with a process that can serve as a 'time-marker'; and after delayed neuronal adequacy is achieved, there is a subjective referral of the sensory experience backwards in time so as to coincide with this initial ' time-marking'.

Why “Sounds Are Judged Longer than Lights”: Application of a Model of the Internal Clock in Humans

Overall, the work showed how a clock model consistent with scalar timing theory, the leading account of animal timing, can address an issue derived from the classical literature on human time perception.