Corpus ID: 22322435

Could both be right? Children's and adults' sensitivity to subjectivity in language

@article{Foushee2017CouldBB,
  title={Could both be right? Children's and adults' sensitivity to subjectivity in language},
  author={Ruthe Foushee and M. Srinivasan},
  journal={Cognitive Science},
  year={2017}
}
While some word meanings, like “spotted,” depend on intersubjectively accessible properties of the world, others like “pretty” invoke speakers’ subjective beliefs. We explored children and adults’ sensitivity to the subjectivity of a range of adjectives, including words like “spotted” and “pretty,” but also words like “tall,” which are evaluated relative to a standard. Participants saw two speakers who had independently experienced sets of exemplars of a novel object kind disagree about whether… Expand

Figures and Topics from this paper

How adjective ordering preferences develop in children
In English, it sounds much better to say “the small grey kitten” than “the grey small kitten.” This is because adult English speakers have robust preferences for ordering adjectives. Recent researchExpand
Faultless disagreement judgments track adults' estimates of population-level consensus over adjective-referent pairs
TLDR
This work has shown a positive relationship between consensus estimates and faultless disagreement judgments: lower consensus and crosslinguistic adjective ordering preferences, which have been explained in terms of adjectives’ subjectivity. Expand
Little Lexical Learners : Quantitatively Assessing the Development of Adjective O rdering Preferences
Adults have robust ordering preferences that determine the relative order of adjectives in multi-adjective strings: this is why small grey kitten and nice round penny are preferable to grey smallExpand
Rollercoasters are not Fun for Mary: Against Indexical Contextualism
Indexical contextualism (IC) is an account of predicates of personal taste (PPTs) which views the semantic content of PPTs as sensitive to the context in which they are uttered, by virtue of theirExpand
Quantitatively Assessing the Development of Adjective Ordering Preferences Using Child-directed and Child-produced Speech Corpora
Using corpus analysis and quantitative approaches, we can see when more abstract underlying representations emerge for adjective ordering preferences: around age 4 It remains unclear whenExpand
A Quantitative Framework for Specifying Underlying Representations in Child Language Acquisition
TLDR
This dissertation shows one quantitatively precise way to identify the nature of developing mental representations in a variety of domains; the approach utilizes the connection between a learners input, creation of a potential mental representation from that input, and evaluation with respect to the learners output. Expand
On the grammatical source of adjective ordering preferences
Scontras et al. (2017) present experimental evidence demonstrating that the best predictor of adjective ordering preferences in the English noun phrase is the subjectivity of the property named byExpand

References

SHOWING 1-10 OF 10 REFERENCES
Meaning and Context in Children's Understanding of Gradable Adjectives
TLDR
It is shown that by 3 years of age, children are sensitive to all three varieties of context– meaning interaction and that their knowledge of this relation with the definite description is appropriately guided by the semantic representations of the GA appearing in it. Expand
The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: an empirical examination.
TLDR
3-year-olds avoided 2 names for an object when the requester for the referent of a second label in a different language was bilingual and present during naming, but not when the speaker was bilingual but absent or monolingual. Expand
What is "high"? The development of reference points for "high" and "low".
TLDR
The results suggest that the categorical usage of relative terms involves a rich and intricate knowledge system and that it takes children considerable time to acquire and organize the relevant pieces of knowledge. Expand
Perspective taking and theory of mind: do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers' knowledge?
TLDR
Children's ability to vary their responses on perspective-taking tasks as a function of the other person's age was examined to study asymmetry in performance on the general knowledge task and the restricted view task. Expand
Compositionality and statistics in adjective acquisition: 4-year-olds interpret tall and short based on the size distributions of novel noun referents.
TLDR
4-year-olds deploy a compositional semantics that is sensitive to statistics and mediated by linguistic labels, and when distractors differed in name and surface features from targets, judgments did not shift. Expand
Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds.
TLDR
Children not only inferred that another person held a desire, but also recognized how desires are related to emotions and understood something about the subjectivity of these desires. Expand
Subjectivity Predicts Adjective Ordering Preferences
From English to Hungarian to Mokilese, speakers exhibit strong ordering preferences in multi-adjective strings: “the big blue box” sounds far more natural than “the blue big box.” We show that anExpand
Negotiating Taste
Using a vague predicate can make commitments about the appropriate use of that predicate in the remaining part of the discourse. For instance, if I assert that some particular pig is fat, I amExpand
Children’s reasoning about unconventional opinions as evidence for naive realism
  • Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.
  • 2015
Naive Realism in Everyday Life: Implications for Social Conflict and Misunderstanding