Chromatic edges, surfaces and constancies in cerebral achromatopsia

@article{Kentridge2004ChromaticES,
  title={Chromatic edges, surfaces and constancies in cerebral achromatopsia},
  author={Robert W. Kentridge and Charles A. Heywood and Alan Cowey},
  journal={Neuropsychologia},
  year={2004},
  volume={42},
  pages={821-830}
}

Figures from this paper

Pupillary responses to coloured and contourless displays in total cerebral achromatopsia.

Although chromatic processing still occurs in cortical colour blindness its function is solely to give rise to the detection of sharp boundaries which, in their case, can provide the perception of shape but not hue.

Color contrast processing in human striate cortex

A patient whose right striate cortex had been removed was tested and it was demonstrated that he made no use of color contrast in judging color appearance but instead made judgments based simply on wavelength comparison.

Lightness computation by the human visual system

  • M. Rudd
  • Biology
    J. Electronic Imaging
  • 2017
Arguments are presented to support the claim that these various visual processes must be instantiated by a particular underlying neural architecture, and it is suggested that edge classification involves a top-down gating of neural edge responses in early visual cortex while spatial integration windowing occurs in cortical area V4 or beyond.

Edge integration in achromatic color perception and the lightness-darkness asymmetry.

  • M. Rudd
  • Biology
    Journal of vision
  • 2013
The neural model is shown to be capable of unifying the quantitative Laws of edge integration in lightness perception with the laws governing brightness, including Stevens' power law brightness model, and makes novel predictions about the quantitative laws governing induced darkness.

The paradox of colour constancy: Plotting the lower borders of perception

This paper resolves a paradox concerning colour constancy. On the one hand, our intuitive, pre-theoretical concept holds that colour constancy involves invariance in the perceived colours of surfaces

Translucence perception is not dependent on cortical areas critical for processing colour or texture ☆

Translucence is an important property of natural materials, and human observers are adept at perceiving changes in translucence. Perceptions of di ff erent material properties appear to arise from di

Color experience in blindsight?

It is argued here that a recent study in which color phosphenes were induced in a blindsighter using bilateral transcranial magnetic stimulation indicates that computations necessary for conscious color vision are lost in blindsight.

Separate channels for processing form, texture, and color: evidence from FMRI adaptation and visual object agnosia.

It is found that areas selective for shape, texture, and color individually were quite distinct from those that respond to all of these features together (shape and texture and color), and appear to correspond to those associated with the perception of complex stimuli such as faces and places.
...

References

SHOWING 1-10 OF 25 REFERENCES

On the role of parvocellular (P) and magnocellular (M) pathways in cerebral achromatopsia.

The patient was singularly proficient at detecting coloured forms in static or dynamic displays even when the saturation of the colours of which the form was composed were such that they were rendered invisible when concealed as a single square in a checkerboard, implying that signals about chroma are still available in extracting shape.

Chromatic Discrimination in a Cortically Colour Blind Observer

Photopic spectral sensitivity showed evidence both for activity of three cone channels and for chromatic opponent processing, indicating that postreceptoral chromatic processing is occurring despite the absence of any conscious awareness of colour.

Insights into the different exploits of colour in the visual cortex

The results suggest that chromatic signals are processed independently to generate perceived object colour or to construct spatially structured objects, and that these functions involve different neural substrates.

Behavioural and electrophysiological chromatic and achromatic contrast sensitivity in an achromatopsic patient.

The results show that information about chromatic contrast is present in some cortical areas, and coded in a colour-opponent fashion, in the absence of any perceptual experience of colour.

Discrimination of cone contrast changes as evidence for colour constancy in cerebral achromatopsia

It is reported that a cerebrally achromatopsic observer, MS, displayed evidence of colour constancy in asymmetric colour matching tasks and was able to discriminate changes in cone ratios for simple, but not complex scenes.

Color contrast in macaque V1.

The neural basis for spatial color contrast and temporal color contrast in primary visual cortex (V1) of the alert macaque is explored and the remarkable degree of specialization shown by cells in V1, especially that of the double-opponent color cells, is discussed.

The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey

This work re-evaluated color processing in V1 by studying single-neuron responses to luminance and to equiluminant color patterns equated for cone contrast, and found that most color-luminance cells are spatial-frequency tuned, with approximately equal selectivity for chromatic and achromatic patterns.

The neurological basis of conscious color perception in a blind patient.

It is concluded that a partly defective color system operating on its own in a severely damaged brain is able to mediate a conscious experience of color in the virtually total absence of other visual abilities.

Spatial Structure of Cone Inputs to Color Cells in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex (V-1)

  • B. Conway
  • Biology
    The Journal of Neuroscience
  • 2001
Red–green (or red–cyan) cells, along with blue–yellow and black–white cells, establish three chromatic axes that are sufficient to describe all of color space.